Newer
Older
Digital_Repository / Misc / Mass downloads / UTas / 2277.html
<!DOCTYPE html PUBLIC "-//W3C//DTD XHTML 1.0 Transitional//EN"
"http://www.w3.org/TR/xhtml1/DTD/xhtml1-transitional.dtd">
<html>
  <head>
    <title>UTas ePrints - Attention is Modulated by Motivational Relevance: A Behavioural and ERP Investigation of Affective Picture Processing</title>
    <script type="text/javascript" src="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/javascript/auto.js"><!-- padder --></script>
    <style type="text/css" media="screen">@import url(http://eprints.utas.edu.au/style/auto.css);</style>
    <style type="text/css" media="print">@import url(http://eprints.utas.edu.au/style/print.css);</style>
    <link rel="icon" href="/images/eprints/favicon.ico" type="image/x-icon" />
    <link rel="shortcut icon" href="/images/eprints/favicon.ico" type="image/x-icon" />
    <link rel="Top" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/" />
    <link rel="Search" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/search" />
    <meta content="Briggs, Kate Elizabeth" name="eprints.creators_name" />
<meta content="thesis" name="eprints.type" />
<meta content="2007-10-22 01:30:05" name="eprints.datestamp" />
<meta content="2008-01-08 15:30:00" name="eprints.lastmod" />
<meta content="show" name="eprints.metadata_visibility" />
<meta content="Attention is Modulated by Motivational
Relevance: A Behavioural and ERP Investigation
of Affective Picture Processing" name="eprints.title" />
<meta content="unpub" name="eprints.ispublished" />
<meta content="380100" name="eprints.subjects" />
<meta content="public" name="eprints.full_text_status" />
<meta content="ERP evidence of affective picture processing generally agrees with one of two dominant
theories. The first is that enhanced ERP responses to pleasant and unpleasant stimuli
relative to neutral reflects the processing of stimulus motivational relevance, referred to
as the quadratic effect, and the second is that enhanced ERP responses to unpleasant
stimuli compared to pleasant and neutral stimuli reflects a negativity bias. The
overarching aims of the current series of empirical studies were to identify which of the
two aforementioned theories can most definitively account for affective picture
processing (Phase 1); and to investigate how processes of attentional engagement and
disengagement are influenced by the presence of motivationally relevant stimuli (Phase
2). Pictorial affective stimuli (high and low arousing pleasant, unpleasant, sexual, and
neutral stimuli) were presented in a modified oddball paradigm to 38 participants (19
male, 19 female) in Experiment 1 and 34 participants (17 male, 17 female) in Experiment
2. A negativity bias was demonstrated for P3b amplitude in Experiment 1; however
significantly enhanced P3b amplitudes evoked in response to sexually explicit stimuli in
Experiment 2 was not consistent with either dominant theory, and raised questions as to
the separable effects of motivational relevance and sexual arousal on cognitive processes.
Experiment 3 was aimed at investigating whether ERP responses are differentially
modulated by the social content of affective picture stimuli. The same participants from
Experiment 2 participated in Experiment 3 and the oddball task involved the presentation
of low arousing social and non-social pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral stimuli. No
significant differences in ERP component measures were shown between social and nonsocial
pleasant, or between social and non-social unpleasant stimuli, however both P2 and
P3b component amplitudes were enhanced in response to neutral faces compared with
neutral objects. Factors associated with facial recognition and difficulties extracting
affective information from a somewhat ambiguous neutral expression were cited as
possible explanations for the observed ERP component modulations.
The principal aim of Phase 2 was to investigate whether the presentation of
appetitive and aversive cues influences the engagement and disengagement components
of covert visual attention as inferred by responses to validly and invalidly cued targets
respectively. Participants in Experiment 4 @=I9 female) and Experiment 5 @=I8
female) were presented with a modified peripheral cueing paradigm, where pictorial
stimuli (sexual, mutilation, threatening, and neutral) served as peripheral cues. Target
processing as indexed by P1 and P3b amplitude showed significant facilitation in both
Experiments 4 and 5 when targets were cued by sexual and mutilation stimuli, regardless
of whether cueing was valid or invalid. It was therefore concluded that the engagement
and disengagement components of covert visual attention are not differentially affected
by motivationally relevant cues; rather, normal participants demonstrate a global
response bias when respondmg to targets that are cued by motivationally relevant
appetitive and aversive cues. The same participants from Experiment 5 were presented
with a peripheral cueing paradigm in Experiment 6, which aimed to investigate the effect
of phylogenetically (biological) and ontogenetically (cultural) fear-relevant stimuli on
processes of covert visual attention. Pictorial stimuli depicting dangerous animals, human
threat, and neutral objects served as peripheral cues. In line with preparedness theory
(Seligman 1970, 1971), target processing was facilitated by the presence of animal threat
stimuli compared to human threat and neutral stimuli, and also the early level of visual
processing as indexed by cue-evoked P 1 amplitude was enhanced in response to
phylogenetically, fear-relevant animal stimuli. A global response bias was again
demonstrated in Experiment 6, and it was concluded that the attentional system of normal
participants is sensitive to stimuli that have been evolutionarily associated with threat
and/or fear. The current dissertation therefore has theoretical implications for the
systematic study of affective picture processing. Furthermore, the introduction of a
peripheral cueing paradigm to the study of affective picture processing provides a new
insight into the effect that both appetitive and aversive stimuli have on processes of
attentional orienting and target processing." name="eprints.abstract" />
<meta content="2007-06" name="eprints.date" />
<meta content="published" name="eprints.date_type" />
<meta content="307" name="eprints.pages" />
<meta content="University of Tasmania" name="eprints.institution" />
<meta content="Psychology" name="eprints.department" />
<meta content="phd" name="eprints.thesis_type" />
<meta content="Adolphs, R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinions in
Neurobiology, 12, 169-177.
Aftanas, L., Varlamov, A., Pavlov, S., Makhnev, V., &amp; Reva, N. (2001). Event-related
synchronization and desynchronization during affective processing: Emergence
of valence-related time-dependent hemispheric asymmetries in theta and upper
alpha band. International Journal of Neuroscience, 110, 197-219.
Amir, N., Elias, J., Klumpp, H., &amp; Prezeworski, A. (2003). Attentional bias to threat in
social phobia: Facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention
from threat? Behaviour and Research Therapy, 41, 1325-1335.
Amrhein, C., Mühlberger, A., Pauli, P., &amp; Wiedemann, G. (2004). Modulation of eventrelated
brain potentials during affective picture processing: A complement to
startle reflex and skin conductance response? International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 54, 231-240.
Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T., Canavan, A. G. M., &amp; Rockstroh, B. (1990). Slow potentials
of the cerebral cortex and behavior. Physiological Reviews, 79, 1-41.
Bledowski, C., Prvulovic, D., Goebel, R., Zanella, F. E., &amp; Linden, D. E. J. (2004).
Attentional systems in target and distractor processing: A combined ERP and
fMRI study. NeuroImage, 22, 530-540.
Bousten, L., Humphreys, G. W., Praamstra, P., &amp; Warbrick, T. (2006). Comparing
neural correlates of configural processing in faces and objects: An ERP study of
the Thatcher illusion. NeuroImage, 32, 352-367.
Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In J. Cacioppo, T. L. G. Tassinary &amp;
G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 602-642).
USA: Cambridge University Press.
241
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Sabatienlli, D., &amp; Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and
motivation II: Sex differences in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 3000-3319.
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B., N, &amp; Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and
motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion,
1, 276-298.
Bradley, M. M., &amp; Lang, P. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: Behavior, feeling, and
physiology. In R. D. Lane &amp; L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Bradley, M. M., Sabatienlli, D., Lang, P. J., Fitzsimmons, J., R, King, W., &amp; Desai, P.
(2003). Activation of the visual cortex in motivated attention. Behavioural
Neuroscience, 117, 369-380.
Budo, G., Sarlo, M., &amp; Palomba, D. (2002). Attentional resources measured by reaction
times highlight differences within pleasant and unpleasant, high arousing stimuli.
Motivation and Emotion, 26, 123-137.
Cacioppo, J., T, &amp; Berntson, G., G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and
evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive
and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 401-423.
Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., Gardner, W. L., &amp; Berntson, G. G. (1994). Bioelectrical
echoes from evaluative categorizations: I a late positive brain potential that
varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 67, 115-125.
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., &amp; Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar
conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative space
model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3-25.
242
Canli, T., Desmond, J. E., Zhao, Z., &amp; Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Hemispheric asymmetry
for emotional stimuli detected with fMRI. Neuroreport, 9, 3233-3239.
Carretié, L., Hinojosa, J. A., Martín-Loeches, M., Mercardo, F., &amp; Tapia, M. (2004).
Automatic attention to emotional stimuli: Neural correlates. Human Brain
Mapping, 22, 290-299.
Carretié, L., Iglesias, T., Garcia, M., &amp; Ballesteros, M. (1996). N300, P300 and the
emotional processing of visual stimuli. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Neurophysiology, 103, 298-303.
Carretié, L., Iglesias, T., &amp; Garcia, T. (1997). A study on the emotional processing of
visual stimuli through event-related potentials. Brain and Cognition, 34, 207-
217.
Carretié, L., Martín-Loeches, M., Hinojosa, J. A., &amp; Mercardo, F. (2001b). Emotion and
attention interactions studies through event-related potentials. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 1109-1128.
Carretié, L., Mercardo, F., Tapia, M., &amp; Hinojosa, J. A. (2001a). Emotion, attention, and
the 'negativity bias', studied through event-related potentials. International
Journal of Psychophysiology, 41, 75-85.
Clark, V. P., &amp; Hillyard, S. A. (1996). Spatial selective attention affects early
extrastriate but not specific striate components of the visual evoked potential.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 387-402.
Collie, A., Maruff, P., Yucel, M., Currie, J., &amp; Danckert, J. (2000). Spatiotemporal
distribution of facilitation and inhibition of return arising from the reflexive
orienting of covert attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 26, 1733-1745.
243
Comerchero, M. D., &amp; Polich, J. (1999). P3a and P3b from typical auditory and visual
stimuli. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 24-30.
Cook, M., &amp; Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant
versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 98, 448-459.
Cook, M., &amp; Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning
of fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 16, 372-389.
Courchesne, E., Hillyard, S. A., &amp; Galambos, R. (1975). Stimulus novelty, task
relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. Electroencephalography and
Clinical Neurophysiology, 39, 131-143.
Crites, S. L., Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., &amp; Berntson, G. G. (1995). Bioelectrical
echoes from evaluative categorization: II A late positive brain potential that
varies as a function of attitude registration rather than attitude report. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology.
Crowley, K. E., &amp; Colrain, I. M. (2004). A review of the evidence for P2 being an
independent component process: Age, sleep, and modality. Clinical
Neurophysiology, 115, 732-744.
Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, N., &amp; Lang, P. J. (2000).
Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Covariation with autonomic
arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology, 52, 95-111.
Damasio, A. R. (1995). Toward a neurobiology of emotion and feeling: Operational
concepts and hypotheses. The Neuroscientist, 1, 19-25.
Davidson, R. J. (1992). Emotion and affective style: Hemispheric substrates.
Psychological Science, 3, 39-43.
244
Davidson, R. J. (1993a). Cerebral asymmetry and emotion: Conceptual and
methodological conundrums. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 115-138.
Davidson, R. J. (1993b). Parsing affective space: Perspectives from neuropsychology
and psychophysiology. Neuropsychology, 7, 464-475.
Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., &amp; Friesen, W. (1990).
Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain
physiology I. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 330-341.
de Jong, P. J., Merckelbach, H., &amp; Arntz, A. (1995). Covariation bias in phobic women:
The relationship between a priori expectancy, on-line expectancy, autonomic
responding, and a posteriori contingency judgement. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 104, 55-62.
De Silva, P., Rachman, S. S., &amp; Seligman, M. P. E. (1977). Prepared phobias and
obsessions: Therapeutic outcome. Behaviour and Research Therapy, 15, 65-77.
Decon, D., Breton, F., Ritter, W., &amp; Vaughan, H. G. (1991). The relationship between
N2 and N400: Scalp distribution, stimulus probability, and task relevance.
Psychophysiology, 28, 185-200.
Delplanque, S., Lavoie, M. E., Hot, P., Silvert, L., &amp; Sequeira, H. (2004). Modulation of
cognitive processing by emotional valence studied through event-related
potentials in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 365, 1-4.
Delplanque, S., Silvert, L., Hot, P., Rigoulot, S., &amp; Sequeira, H. (2006). Arousal and
valence effects on event-related P3a and P3b during emotional categorization.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 315-322.
Delplanque, S., Silvert, L., Hot, P., &amp; Sequeira, H. (2005). Event-related P3a and P3b in
response to unpredictable emotional stimuli. Biological Psychology, 68, 107-120.
245
Demaree, H. A., Everhart, D. E., Youngstrom, E. A., &amp; Harrison, D. W. (2005). Brain
lateralization of emotional processing: Historical roots and a future incorporating
&quot;dominance&quot;. Behavioural and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 4, 3-20.
Derryberry, D., &amp; Reed, M. A. (1996). Regulatory processes and the development of
cognitive representations. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 215-235.
Derryberry, D., &amp; Tucker, D. M. (1992). Neural mechanisms of emotion. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 329-338.
Diedrich, O., Naumann, E., Maier, S., Becker, G., &amp; Bartussek, D. (1997). A frontal
slow wave in the ERP associated with emotional slides. Journal of
Psychophysiology, 11, 71-84.
Dien, J., Spencer, K. M., &amp; Donchin, E. (2004). Parsing the late positive complex:
Mental chronometry and the ERP components that inhabit the neighbourhood of
the P300. Psychophysiology, 41, 665-678.
Donchin, E. (1981). Surprise!...surprise? Psychophysiology, 18, 491-513.
Donchin, E., &amp; Coles, M. G. G. (1988). Is the P300 component a manifestation of
context updating? Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 11, 357-374.
Dunn, B. R., Dunn, D. A., Languis, M., &amp; Andrews, D. (1998). The relation of ERP
components to complex memory processing. Brain and Cognition, 36, 355-376.
Eimer, M. (1996). ERP modulations indicate the selective processing of visual stimuli as
a result of transient and sustained spatial attention. Psychophysiology, 33, 13-21.
Eimer, M. (2000). Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved
in face perception and recognition. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111, 694-705.
Eimer, M., &amp; Holmes, A. (2002). An ERP study on the time course of emotional face
processing. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 13, 1-5.
246
Ekman, P., &amp; Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Consulting Psychology
Press, Plato Alto, CA.
Empson, J. (1986). Human Brain Waves: The Psychological Significance of the
Electroencephalogram. New York: Stockon Press.
Esteves, F., Parra, C., Dimberg, U., &amp; Öhman, A. (1994). Nonconscious associative
learning: Pavlovian conditioning of skin conductance responses to masked fearrelevant
facial stimuli. Psychophysiology, 31, 375-385.
Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., &amp; Coles, M. G. H. (2000). Event-related brain potentials:
methods, theory, and applications. In J. Cacioppo, T. L. G. Tassinary &amp; G..
Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed.) (pp. 53-84). USA:
Cambridge University Press.
Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowels, R., &amp; Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or
hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 130, 681-700.
Friedmann, D., Cycowitz, M. Y., &amp; Gaeta, H. (2001). The novelty P3: An event-related
brain potential (ERP) sign on the brain's evaluation of novelty. Neuroscience and
Behavioural Reviews, 25, 355-373.
Gaeta, H., Friedmann, D., &amp; Hunt, G. (2003). Stimulus characteristics and task category
dissociate the anterior and posterior aspects of the novelty P3. Psychophysiology,
40, 198-208.
Gainotti, G. (2000). Neuropsychological theories of emotion. In J. C. Borod (Ed.), The
Neuropsychology of Emotion (pp. 214-235). Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
247
García-Larrea, L., Lukaszewicz, A-C., &amp; Mauguiere, F. (1992). Revisiting the oddball
paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional
effects. Neuropsychologia, 30, 723-741.
Gehring, W. J., Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., &amp; Donchin, E. (1992). Probability effects
on stimulus evaluation and response processes. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 198-216.
George, M. S., Ketter, T. A., Parekh, P. I., Herscovitch, P., &amp; Post, R. M. (1996).
Gender differences in regional cerebral blood flow during transient self-induced
sadness or happiness. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 859-871.
Georgiou, G., A, Bleakley, C., Hayward, J., Russo, R., Dutton, K., Eltiti, S., et al.
(2005). Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional faces.
Visual Cognition, 12, 145-158.
Goldstein, A., Spencer, K. M., &amp; Donchin, E. (2002). The influence of stimulus
deviance and novelty on the P300 and Novelty P3. Psychophysiology, 39, 781-
790.
Guttman, L. (1954). A new approach to factor analysis: The new radix. In P. R.
Lazarsfeld (Ed.), Mathematical thinking in the social sciences. Glencore: Free
Press.
Hagemann, D., Hewig, J., Naumann, E., Seifert, J., &amp; Bartussek, D. (2005). Resting
brain asymmetry and affective reactivity: Aggregated data support the righthemisphere
hypothesis. Journal of Individual Differences, 26, 139-154.
Hamm, A. O., Schupp, H., T., &amp; Weike, A., I. (2003). Motivational organization of
emotions: Autonomic changes, cortical responses, and reflex modulation. In R. J.
Davidson, K. R. Scherer &amp; H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective
science (pp. 187-212). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
248
Heller, W. (1990). The neuropsychology of emotion: Developmental patterns and
implications form psychopathology. In N. Stein, B. L. Leventhal &amp; T. Trabasso
(Eds.), Psychological and biological approaches to emotion (pp. 167-211).
Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
Heller, W. (1993). Neuropsychological mechanisms of individual differences in
emotion, personality, and arousal. Neuropsychology, 7, 476-489.
Henriques, J. B., &amp; Davidson, R. J. (1990). Regional brain electrical asymmetries
discriminate between previously depressed and healthy controls. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 99, 22-31.
Henriques, J. B., &amp; Davidson, R. J. (1991). Left frontal hypo-activation in depression.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 535-545.
Hillman, C. H., Rosengren, K. S., &amp; Smith, D. (2003). Emotion and motivated behavior:
Postural adjustments to affective picture viewing. Biological Psychology, in
press, 1-12.
Hillyard, S. A., Luck, S. J., &amp; Mangun, G. R. (1994). The cuing of attention to visual
field locations: Analysis with ERP recordings. In H. J. Heinze, T. F. Munte &amp; G.
R. Mangun (Eds.), Cognitive electrophysiology (pp. 1-25). Boston: Birhauser.
Holmes, A., Vuilleumier, P., &amp; Eimer, M. (2003). The processing of emotional facial
expression is gated by spatial attention: Evidence from event-related brain
potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 174-184.
Hopfinger, J., &amp; Mangun, G. R. (1998). Reflexive attention modulates processing of
visual stimuli in the human extrastriate cortex. Psychological Science, 9, 441-
447.
249
Hopfinger, J. B., &amp; Mangun, G. R. (2001). Tracking the influence of reflexive attention
on sensory and cognitive processing. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral
Neuroscience, 1, 56-65.
Hopfinger, J., &amp; Ries, A. (2005). Automatic versus contingent mechanisms of sensorydriven
neural biasing and reflexive attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
17, 1341-1352.
Hugdahl, K., &amp; Johnsen, B. H. (1989). Preparedness and electrodermal fearconditioning:
Ontogenetic vs phylogenetic explanations. Behaviour and
Research Therapy, 27, 269-278.
Hugdahl, K., &amp; Karker, A.-C. (1981). Biological vs experimental factors in phobic
conditioning. Behaviour and Research Therapy, 19, 109-115.
Ito, T. A., &amp; Cacioppo, J. T. (2005). Variations on a human universal: Individual
differences in positivity offset and negativity bias. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 1-
26.
Ito, T. A., Cacioppo, J. T., &amp; Lang, P. J. (1998). Eliciting affect using the international
affective picture system: Trajectories through evaluative space. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 855-879.
Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., &amp; Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more
heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887-900.
Jasper, H. H. (1958). The 10-20 electrode system of the international federation.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371-375..
Johnson Jr, R. (1986). A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology, 23, 367-
383.
250
Johnson Jr, R. (1993). On the neural generators of the P300 component of the eventrelated
potential. Psychophysiology, 30, 90-97.
Johnston, V. S., Miller, D. R., &amp; Burleson, M. H. (1986). Multiple P3s to emotional
stimuli and their theoretical significance. Psychophysiology, 23, 684-693.
Jones, N. A., &amp; Fox, N. A. (1992). Electroencephalogram asymmetry during
emotionally evocative films and its relation to positive and negative affectivity.
Brain and Cognition, 20, 280-299.
Junghöfer, M., Bradley, M. M., Elbert, T., &amp; Lang, P. J. (2001). Fleeting images: A new
look at early emotion discrimination. Psychophysiology, 38, 175-178.
Karama, S., Lecours, A. R., Lerox, J-M., Bourgouin, P., Beaudoin, G., Jourbert, S., et al.
(2002). Areas of brain activation in males and females during viewing of erotic
film excerpts. Human Brain Mapping, 16-13.
Katayma, J., &amp; Polich, J. (1998). Stimulus context determines the P3a and P3b.
Psychophysiology, 35, 23-33.
Kayser, J., Tenke, C., Nordby, H., Hammerborg, D., Hugdahl, K., &amp; Erdmann, G.
(1997). Event-related potential (ERP) asymmetries to emotional stimuli in a
visual half-field paradigm. Psychophysiology, 34, 414-426.
Keil, A., Bradley, M. M., Hauk, O., Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., &amp; Lang, P. J. (2002).
Large-scale correlates of affective picture processing. Psychophysiology, 39,
641-649.
Kemp, A. H., Silberstein, R. B., Armstrong, S. M., &amp; Nathan, P. J. (2004). Gender
differences in the cortical electrophysiological processing of visual emotional
stimuli. NeuroImage, 21, 632-646.
Kenedy, S. J., Rapee, R. M., &amp; Mazurski, J. (1997). Covariation bias for phylogenetic vs
ontogenetic fear-relevant stimuli. Behaviour and Research Therapy, 35, 415-422.
251
Kenntner-Mabials, R., &amp; Pauli, P. (2005). Affective modulation of brain potentials to
painful and nonpainful stimuli. Psychophysiology, 42, 559-567.
Kok, A. (1997). Event-related potential (ERP) reflections of mental resources: A review
and synthesis. Biological Psychology, 45, 19-56.
Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity.
Psychophysiology, 38, 557-577.
Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain: an interdisciplinary approach.
Chicago: Chicago University Press
Kopp, B., &amp; Altmann, R. (2005). Neurocognitive effects of phobia-related stimuli in
animal-fearful individuals. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioural Neuroscience,
5, 373-387.
Kutas, M., McCarthy, G., &amp; Donchin, E. (1977). Augmenting mental chronometry: The
P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. Science, 197, 792-795.
Lane, R. D., Chua, P. M. L., &amp; Dolan, R. .J. (1999). Common effects of emotional
valence, arousal and attention on neural activation during visual processing of
pictures. Neuropsychologia, 37, 989-997.
Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe. American Psychologist, 50, 372-385.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., &amp; Cuthbert, B., N. (1990). Emotion, attention, and the
startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97, 377-395.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., &amp; Cuthbert, B. N. (1992). A motivational analysis of
emotion: Reflex-cortex connections. Psychological Science, 3, 44-49.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., &amp; Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect,
activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons &amp; M. T. Balaban (Eds.),
Attention and orienting: sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97-135).
Manhwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaun Associates Publishers.
252
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., &amp; Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). University of Florida Patent No.
Technical Report A-4, The Centre for Research in Psychophysiology.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., Fitzsimmons, J. R., Cuthbert, B. N., Scott, J. D., Moulder,
B., et al. (1998). Emotional arousal and activation of the visual cortex: An fMRI
analysis. Psychophysiology, 32, 199-210.
Lang, P. J., Davis, M., &amp; Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Animal models and
human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 137-159.
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., &amp; Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at
pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioural reactions. Psychophysiology,
30, 261-273.
Lavric, A., Pizzagalli, D. A., &amp; Forstmeier, S. (2004). When 'go' and 'nogo' are equally
frequent: ERP components and cortical tomography. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 20, 2483-2488.
Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition.
American Psychologist, 37, 1019-1024.
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39, 124-
129.
LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interactions in the brain. Cognition and
Emotion, 3, 267-289.
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23,
155-184.
Leventhal, H., &amp; Scherer, K. (1987). The relationship of emotion to cognition: A
functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 3-28.
Lewis, M. D., &amp; Stieben, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in the brain: Conceptual issues
and directions for developmental research. Child Development, 75, 371-376
253
Livesey, P. J. (1986). Learning and emotion: A biological synthesis (Vol. 1). Hillsdale,
N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Loew, A., Bradley, M. M., Keller, A. R., &amp; Lang, P. J. (2003). Emotional arousal and
novelty: Effects of within-session repetition on ERPs and skin conductance.
Psychophysiology, S55.
Mangun, G. R. (1995). Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention.
Psychophysiology, 32, 4-18.
Mangun, G. R., &amp; Hillyard, S. A. (1991). Mechanisms and models of selective attention.
In M. D. Rugg &amp; M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Electrophysiology of mind. Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Maylor, E. A. (1985). Facilitatory and inhibitory components of orienting in visual
space. In M. I. Posner &amp; O. S. M. Marin (Eds.), Attention and Performance XI
(pp. 189-204). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Association.
McDonald, J. J., Ward, L. M., &amp; Kiehl, K. A. (1999). An event-related brain potential
study of inhibition of return. Perception and psychophysics, 61, 1411-1423.
McManis, M. H., Bradley, M. M., Berg, W. K., Cuthbert, B. N., &amp; Lang, P. J. (2001).
Emotional reactions in children: Verbal, physiological, and behavioural
responses to affective pictures. Psychophysiology, 38, 222-231.
Meinhardt, J., &amp; Pekrun, R. (2003). Attentional resource allocation to emotional events:
An ERP study. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 477-500.
Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In J. M. Hunt (Ed.), Personality
and the behavioural disorders: A handbook based on experimental clinical
research. USA: The Ronald Press Company.
254
Miller, N. E. (1959). Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extensions to conflict
behavior, motivation, and social learning. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study
of a science. General systematic formulations, learning, and special processes.
USA: McGraw-Hill Book Company.
Mineka, S., &amp; Öhman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The selective, automatic,
and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 927-937.
Mini, A., Palomba, D., &amp; Bravi, S. (1996). Emotional information processing and visual
evoked brain potentials. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 143-152.
Mourão-Miranda, J., Volchan, E., Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Oliveira, L., Bramati,
I., et al. (2003). Contributions of stimulus valence and arousal to visual
activation during emotional perception. NeuroImage, 20, 1955-1963.
Mühlburger, A., Wiedemann, G., Hermann, M. J., &amp; Pauli, P. (2006). Phylo- and
ontogenetic fears and the expectancy of danger: Differences between spider- and
flight-phobic subjects in cognitive and physiological responses to disorderspecific
stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 380-589.
Müller, H. J., &amp; Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual
attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315-330.
Näätänen, R., Alho, K., &amp; Schroger, E. (2002). Electrophysiology of attention. In H.
Pashler &amp; J. Wixted (Eds.), Steven's handbook of experimental psychology (pp.
601-653). New York: John Wiley &amp; Sons. Inc.
Nelson, C. A., &amp; Nugent, K. M. (1990). Recognition memory and resource allocation as
revealed by children's event-related potential responses to happy and angry faces.
Developmental Psychology, 26, 171-179.
255
Öhman, A. (1986). Face the beast and fear the face: Animal and social fears as
prototypes for evolutionary analyses of emotion. Psychophysiology, 23, 123-145.
Öhman, A., Flykt, A., &amp; Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the
snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466-478.
Öhman, A., Lundqvist, D., &amp; Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: A
threat advantage with schematic faces. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80, 381-396.
Öhman, A., &amp; Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, Phobias, and Preparedness: Toward an evolved
module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483-522.
Öhman, A., &amp; Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent: Snakes as a prototypical
stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 12, 5-9.
Öhman, A., &amp; Soares, J. J. F. (1993). On the automatic nature of phobic fearconditioned
electrodermal responses to masked fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 102, 121-132.
Öhman, A., &amp; Soares, J. J. F. (1998). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli:
Expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant
stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 69-82.
Palomba, D., Angrilli, A., &amp; Mini, A. (1997). Visual evoked potentials, heart rate
responses and memory to emotional pictorial stimuli. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 27, 55-67.
Panksepp, J. (2003). At the interface of the affective, behavioural, and cognitive
neurosciences: Decoding the emotional feelings of the brain. Brain and
Cognition, 52, 4-14.
256
Patterson, D. W., &amp; Schmidt, L. A. (2003). Neuroanatomy of the human affective
system. Brain and Cognition, 52, 24-26.
Perchet, C., &amp; García-Larrea, L. (2000). Visuospatial attention and motor reaction in
children: An electrophysiological study of the &quot;Posner&quot; paradigm.
Psychophysiology, 37, 231-241.
Picton, T. W. (1992). The P300 wave of the human event-related potential. Journal of
Clinical Neurophysiology, 9, 456-479.
Picton, T. W., Bentin, S., Berg, P., Donchin, E., Hillyard, S. A., Johnson Jr, R., et al.
(2000). Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition:
Recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology, 37, 127-152.
Polich, J., &amp; Camerchero, M. D. (2003). P3a from visual stimuli: Typicality, task, and
topography. Brain Topography, 15, 141-152.
Polich, J., &amp; Kok, A. (1995). Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: An
integrative review. Biological Psychology, 41, 103-146.
Pollack, S. D., &amp; Tolley-Schell, S. A. (2003). Selective attention to facial emotion in
physically abused children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 323-383.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 32, 3-25.
Posner, M. I. (1988). Structures and functions of selective attention. In T. Boll &amp; B. K.
Bryant (Eds.), Neurophysiology and brain function: The master lectures series
(Vol. 7, pp. 173-202). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
Posner, M. I., &amp; Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma &amp; T.
U. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X (pp. 531-556). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
257
Posner, M. I., &amp; Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual
Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42.
Posner, M. I., &amp; Rafal, R. D. (1982). Neural systems control spatial orienting.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, 298, 187-
198.
Pourtois, G., Dan, E. S., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., &amp; Vuilleumier, P. (2005). Enhanced
extrastriate visual response to bandpass spatial frequency filtered fearful-faces:
Time course and topographic evoked-potentials mapping. Human Brain
Mapping, 26, 65-79.
Prime, D. J, &amp; Ward, L. M. (2004). Inhibition of return from stimulus to response.
Psychological Science, 15, 272-276.
Prime, D., J, &amp; Ward, L., M. (2006). Cortical expressions of inhibition of return. Brain
Research, 1072, 161-174.
Pritchard, W. S. (1981). Psychophysiology of P300. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 506-
540.
Pritchard, W. S., Brandt, M., &amp; Barratt, E. S. (1986). Analyzing event-related potentials:
The utility of high and low pass filtering in improving the relationship between
various amplitude measures and principal components analysis factor scores.
Psychophysiology, 23, 166-173.
Pury, C. L. S., &amp; Mineka, S. (1997). Covariation bias for blood-injury stimuli and
aversive outcomes. Behaviour and Research Therapy, 35, 35-47.
Rösler, F., &amp; Heil, M. (1991). Toward a functional categorization of slow waves: Taking
into account past and future events. Psychophysiology, 28, 344-358.
Rozin, P., &amp; Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negative dominance, and
contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296-320.
258
Ruchkin, D., Johnson, J. R., Mahaffey, D., &amp; Sutton, S. (1988). Toward a functional
categorization of slow waves. Psychophysiology, 25, 339-353.
Rudell, A. P., &amp; Hua, J. (1995). Recognition potential latency and word image
degradation. Brain and Language, 51, 229-241.
Rushby, J., Barry, R. J., &amp; Doherty, R. (2005). Separation of the components of the late
positive complex in an ERP dishabituation paradigm. Clinical Neurophysiology,
116, 2363-2380.
Russell, J. A. (1979). Affective space is bipolar. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 37, 345-356.
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 39, 1161-1178.
Schneider, F., Habel, U., Kessler, C., Salloum, J. B., &amp; Posse, S. (2000). Gender
differences in regional cerebral activity during sadness. Human Brain Mapping,
9, 226-238.
Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T. A., &amp; Lang, P. J.
(2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by
motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37, 257-261.
Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Hillman, C. H., Hamm, A. O., &amp; Lang,
P. J. (2004). Brain processes in emotional perception: Motivated attention.
Cognition and Emotion, 18, 593-611.
Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., &amp; Hamm, A. O. (2003b). Attention and
emotion: An ERP analysis of facilitated emotional stimulus processing.
NeuroReport, 14, 1-5.
259
Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I, &amp; Hamm, A. O. (2003a). Emotional
facilitation of sensory processing in the visual cortex. Psychological Science, 14,
7-13.
Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I, &amp; Hamm, A. O. (2004). The selective
processing of briefly presented affective pictures: An ERP analysis.
Psychophysiology, 41, 441-449.
Schupp, H., T., Öhman, A., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A., I, Stockburger, J., &amp; Hamm, A.
O. (2004). The facilitated processing of threatening faces: An ERP analysis.
Emotion, 4, 189-200.
Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, N., &amp; Lang, P. J. (1997).
Probe P3 and blinks: Two measures of affective startle modulation.
Psychophysiology, 34, 1-6.
Seifert, J., Naumann, E., Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., &amp; Bartussek, D. (2006). Motivated
executive attention-incentives and the noise compatibility effect. Biological
Psychology, 71, 80-89.
Seligman, M. P. E. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological
Review, 77, 406-418.
Seligman, M. P. E. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307-320.
Simon-Thomas, E. R., Role, K. O., &amp; Knight, R. T. (2005). Behavioural and
electrophysiological evidence of a right hemisphere bias for the influence of
negative emotion on higher cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17,
518-529.
Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 81, 174-176.
260
Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., &amp; Larsen, J. T. (2003). May I have your attention please:
Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41,
171-183.
Speilberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Vagg, P. R., &amp; Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (form Y). Redwood City, California: Mind Garden.
Stormark, H., Morten, K., Nordby, H., &amp; Hugdahl, K. (1995). Attentional shifts to
emotionally charged cues: Behavioural and ERP data. Cognition and Emotion, 9,
507-523.
Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J., &amp; John, E. R. (1965). Evoked potential correlates of
stimulus uncertainty. Science, 150, 1187-1188.
Taylor, M. J. (2002). Non-spatial attentional effects on P1. Clinical Neurophysiology,
113, 1903-1908.
Terry, K. M., Valdes, L. A., &amp; Neill, W. T. (1994). Does &quot;inhibition of return&quot; occur in
discrimination tasks? Perception and Psychophysics, 55, 279-286.
Tipples, J., &amp; Sharma, D. (2000). Orienting to exogenous cues and attentional pictures
reflect separate processes. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 87-97.
Tomarken, A. J., &amp; Mineka, S. (1989). Fear-relevant selective associations and
covariation bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 381-394.
Tomarken, A. J., Sutton, S. K., &amp; Mineka, S. (1995). Fear-relevant illusory correlations:
What types of associations promote judgemental bias. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 104, 312-326.
van de Laar, M. C., Licht, R., Franken, I. H. A., &amp; Hendriks, V. M. (2004). Eventrelated
potentials indicate motivational relevance of cocaine cues in abstinent
cocaine addicts. Psychopharmacology, 177, 121-129.
261
Verleger, R. (1988). Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context
updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3. Behavioural and
Brain Sciences, 11, 343-427.
Wascher, E., &amp; Tipper, S. (2004). Revealing effects of noninformative spatial cues: An
EEG study of inhibition of return. Psychophysiology, 41, 716-728.
Watson, D., &amp; Tellegen, A. (1985). Towards a consensual structure of mood.
Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219-235.
Wolpe, J., &amp; Lang, P. J. (1964). A fear survey schedule for use in behaviour therapy.
Behaviour and Research Therapy, 2, 27-30.
Wrase, J., Klein, S., Gruesser, S. M., Hermann, D., Flor, H., Mann, K., et al. (2003).
Gender differences in the processing of standardized emotional visual stimuli in
humans: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience Letters,
348, 41-45.
Yiend, J., &amp; Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 665-681.
Zafiropoulou, M., &amp; McPherson, F. M. M. (1986). 'Preparedness&quot; and the severity and
outcomes of clinical phobias. Behaviour and Research Therapy, 24, 221-222.
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American
Psychologist, 35, 151-175.
Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117-123." name="eprints.referencetext" />
<meta content="Briggs, Kate Elizabeth (2007) Attention is Modulated by Motivational Relevance: A Behavioural and ERP Investigation of Affective Picture Processing. PhD thesis, University of Tasmania." name="eprints.citation" />
<meta content="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/1/01front_Kbriggs.pdf" name="eprints.document_url" />
<meta content="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/2/02whole_kbriggs.pdf" name="eprints.document_url" />
<link rel="schema.DC" href="http://purl.org/DC/elements/1.0/" />
<meta content="Attention is Modulated by Motivational
Relevance: A Behavioural and ERP Investigation
of Affective Picture Processing" name="DC.title" />
<meta content="Briggs, Kate Elizabeth" name="DC.creator" />
<meta content="380100 Psychology" name="DC.subject" />
<meta content="ERP evidence of affective picture processing generally agrees with one of two dominant
theories. The first is that enhanced ERP responses to pleasant and unpleasant stimuli
relative to neutral reflects the processing of stimulus motivational relevance, referred to
as the quadratic effect, and the second is that enhanced ERP responses to unpleasant
stimuli compared to pleasant and neutral stimuli reflects a negativity bias. The
overarching aims of the current series of empirical studies were to identify which of the
two aforementioned theories can most definitively account for affective picture
processing (Phase 1); and to investigate how processes of attentional engagement and
disengagement are influenced by the presence of motivationally relevant stimuli (Phase
2). Pictorial affective stimuli (high and low arousing pleasant, unpleasant, sexual, and
neutral stimuli) were presented in a modified oddball paradigm to 38 participants (19
male, 19 female) in Experiment 1 and 34 participants (17 male, 17 female) in Experiment
2. A negativity bias was demonstrated for P3b amplitude in Experiment 1; however
significantly enhanced P3b amplitudes evoked in response to sexually explicit stimuli in
Experiment 2 was not consistent with either dominant theory, and raised questions as to
the separable effects of motivational relevance and sexual arousal on cognitive processes.
Experiment 3 was aimed at investigating whether ERP responses are differentially
modulated by the social content of affective picture stimuli. The same participants from
Experiment 2 participated in Experiment 3 and the oddball task involved the presentation
of low arousing social and non-social pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral stimuli. No
significant differences in ERP component measures were shown between social and nonsocial
pleasant, or between social and non-social unpleasant stimuli, however both P2 and
P3b component amplitudes were enhanced in response to neutral faces compared with
neutral objects. Factors associated with facial recognition and difficulties extracting
affective information from a somewhat ambiguous neutral expression were cited as
possible explanations for the observed ERP component modulations.
The principal aim of Phase 2 was to investigate whether the presentation of
appetitive and aversive cues influences the engagement and disengagement components
of covert visual attention as inferred by responses to validly and invalidly cued targets
respectively. Participants in Experiment 4 @=I9 female) and Experiment 5 @=I8
female) were presented with a modified peripheral cueing paradigm, where pictorial
stimuli (sexual, mutilation, threatening, and neutral) served as peripheral cues. Target
processing as indexed by P1 and P3b amplitude showed significant facilitation in both
Experiments 4 and 5 when targets were cued by sexual and mutilation stimuli, regardless
of whether cueing was valid or invalid. It was therefore concluded that the engagement
and disengagement components of covert visual attention are not differentially affected
by motivationally relevant cues; rather, normal participants demonstrate a global
response bias when respondmg to targets that are cued by motivationally relevant
appetitive and aversive cues. The same participants from Experiment 5 were presented
with a peripheral cueing paradigm in Experiment 6, which aimed to investigate the effect
of phylogenetically (biological) and ontogenetically (cultural) fear-relevant stimuli on
processes of covert visual attention. Pictorial stimuli depicting dangerous animals, human
threat, and neutral objects served as peripheral cues. In line with preparedness theory
(Seligman 1970, 1971), target processing was facilitated by the presence of animal threat
stimuli compared to human threat and neutral stimuli, and also the early level of visual
processing as indexed by cue-evoked P 1 amplitude was enhanced in response to
phylogenetically, fear-relevant animal stimuli. A global response bias was again
demonstrated in Experiment 6, and it was concluded that the attentional system of normal
participants is sensitive to stimuli that have been evolutionarily associated with threat
and/or fear. The current dissertation therefore has theoretical implications for the
systematic study of affective picture processing. Furthermore, the introduction of a
peripheral cueing paradigm to the study of affective picture processing provides a new
insight into the effect that both appetitive and aversive stimuli have on processes of
attentional orienting and target processing." name="DC.description" />
<meta content="2007-06" name="DC.date" />
<meta content="Thesis" name="DC.type" />
<meta content="NonPeerReviewed" name="DC.type" />
<meta content="application/pdf" name="DC.format" />
<meta content="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/1/01front_Kbriggs.pdf" name="DC.identifier" />
<meta content="application/pdf" name="DC.format" />
<meta content="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/2/02whole_kbriggs.pdf" name="DC.identifier" />
<meta content="Briggs, Kate Elizabeth (2007) Attention is Modulated by Motivational Relevance: A Behavioural and ERP Investigation of Affective Picture Processing. PhD thesis, University of Tasmania." name="DC.identifier" />
<meta content="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/" name="DC.relation" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/BibTeX/epprod-eprint-2277.bib" title="BibTeX" type="text/plain" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/ContextObject/epprod-eprint-2277.xml" title="OpenURL ContextObject" type="text/xml" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/ContextObject::Dissertation/epprod-eprint-2277.xml" title="OpenURL Dissertation" type="text/xml" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/ContextObject::Journal/epprod-eprint-2277.xml" title="OpenURL Journal" type="text/xml" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/DC/epprod-eprint-2277.txt" title="Dublin Core" type="text/plain" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/DIDL/epprod-eprint-2277.xml" title="DIDL" type="text/xml" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/EndNote/epprod-eprint-2277.enw" title="EndNote" type="text/plain" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/HTML/epprod-eprint-2277.html" title="HTML Citation" type="text/html; charset=utf-8" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/METS/epprod-eprint-2277.xml" title="METS" type="text/xml" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/MODS/epprod-eprint-2277.xml" title="MODS" type="text/xml" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/RIS/epprod-eprint-2277.ris" title="Reference Manager" type="text/plain" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/Refer/epprod-eprint-2277.refer" title="Refer" type="text/plain" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/Simple/epprod-eprint-2277text" title="Simple Metadata" type="text/plain" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/Text/epprod-eprint-2277.txt" title="ASCII Citation" type="text/plain; charset=utf-8" />
<link rel="alternate" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/export/2277/XML/epprod-eprint-2277.xml" title="EP3 XML" type="text/xml" />

  </head>
  <body bgcolor="#ffffff" text="#000000" onLoad="loadRoutine(); MM_preloadImages('images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c5_f2.gif','images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c7_f2.gif','images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c8_f2.gif','images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c9_f2.gif','images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c10_f2.gif','images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c11_f2.gif','images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r6_c4_f2.gif')">
    
    <div class="ep_noprint"><noscript><style type="text/css">@import url(http://eprints.utas.edu.au/style/nojs.css);</style></noscript></div>




<table width="795" border="0" cellspacing="0" cellpadding="0">
  <tr>
    <td><script language="JavaScript1.2">mmLoadMenus();</script>
      <table border="0" cellpadding="0" cellspacing="0" width="795">
        <!-- fwtable fwsrc="eprints_banner_final2.png" fwbase="ePrints_banner.gif" fwstyle="Dreamweaver" fwdocid = "1249563342" fwnested="0" -->
        <tr>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="32" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="104" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="44" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="105" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="41" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="16" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="68" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="68" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="68" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="82" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="69" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="98" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="1" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
        </tr>
        <tr>
          <td colspan="12"><img name="ePrints_banner_r1_c1" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r1_c1.gif" width="795" height="10" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="1" height="10" border="0" alt="" /></td>
        </tr>
        <tr>
          <td rowspan="6"><img name="ePrints_banner_r2_c1" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r2_c1.gif" width="32" height="118" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td rowspan="5"><a href="http://www.utas.edu.au/"><img name="ePrints_banner_r2_c2" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r2_c2.gif" width="104" height="103" border="0" alt="" /></a></td>
          <td colspan="10"><img name="ePrints_banner_r2_c3" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r2_c3.gif" width="659" height="41" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="1" height="41" border="0" alt="" /></td>
        </tr>
        <tr>
          <td colspan="3"><a href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/"><img name="ePrints_banner_r3_c3" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r3_c3.gif" width="190" height="31" border="0" alt="" /></a></td>
          <td rowspan="2" colspan="7"><img name="ePrints_banner_r3_c6" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r3_c6.gif" width="469" height="37" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="1" height="31" border="0" alt="" /></td>
        </tr>
        <tr>
          <td colspan="3"><img name="ePrints_banner_r4_c3" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r4_c3.gif" width="190" height="6" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="1" height="6" border="0" alt="" /></td>
        </tr>
        <tr>
          <td colspan="2"><img name="ePrints_banner_r5_c3" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c3.gif" width="149" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td rowspan="2" colspan="2"><a href="/information.html" onMouseOut="MM_swapImgRestore();MM_startTimeout()" onMouseOver="MM_showMenu(window.mm_menu_0821132634_0,0,25,null,'ePrints_banner_r5_c5');MM_swapImage('ePrints_banner_r5_c5','','/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c5_f2.gif',1);"><img name="ePrints_banner_r5_c5" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c5.gif" width="57" height="25" border="0" alt="About" /></a></td>
          <td rowspan="2"><a href="/view/" onMouseOut="MM_swapImgRestore();MM_startTimeout()" onMouseOver="MM_showMenu(window.mm_menu_0821133021_1,0,25,null,'ePrints_banner_r5_c7');MM_swapImage('ePrints_banner_r5_c7','','/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c7_f2.gif',1);"><img name="ePrints_banner_r5_c7" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c7.gif" width="68" height="25" border="0" alt="Browse" /></a></td>
          <td rowspan="2"><a href="/perl/search/simple" onMouseOut="MM_swapImgRestore();MM_startTimeout()" onMouseOver="MM_showMenu(window.mm_menu_0821133201_2,0,25,null,'ePrints_banner_r5_c8');MM_swapImage('ePrints_banner_r5_c8','','/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c8_f2.gif',1);"><img name="ePrints_banner_r5_c8" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c8.gif" width="68" height="25" border="0" alt="Search" /></a></td>
          <td rowspan="2"><a href="/perl/register" onMouseOut="MM_swapImgRestore();MM_startTimeout();" onMouseOver="MM_showMenu(window.mm_menu_1018171924_3,0,25,null,'ePrints_banner_r5_c9');MM_swapImage('ePrints_banner_r5_c9','','/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c9_f2.gif',1);"><img name="ePrints_banner_r5_c9" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c9.gif" width="68" height="25" border="0" alt="register" /></a></td>
          <td rowspan="2"><a href="/perl/users/home" onMouseOut="MM_swapImgRestore();MM_startTimeout()" onMouseOver="MM_showMenu(window.mm_menu_0821133422_4,0,25,null,'ePrints_banner_r5_c10');MM_swapImage('ePrints_banner_r5_c10','','/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c10_f2.gif',1);"><img name="ePrints_banner_r5_c10" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c10.gif" width="82" height="25" border="0" alt="user area" /></a></td>
          <td rowspan="2"><a href="/help/" onMouseOut="MM_swapImgRestore();MM_startTimeout()" onMouseOver="MM_showMenu(window.mm_menu_0821133514_5,0,25,null,'ePrints_banner_r5_c11');MM_swapImage('ePrints_banner_r5_c11','','/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c11_f2.gif',1);"><img name="ePrints_banner_r5_c11" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c11.gif" width="69" height="25" border="0" alt="Help" /></a></td>
          <td rowspan="3" colspan="4"><img name="ePrints_banner_r5_c12" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r5_c12.gif" width="98" height="40" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="1" height="1" border="0" alt="" /></td>
        </tr>
        <tr>
          <td rowspan="2"><img name="ePrints_banner_r6_c3" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r6_c3.gif" width="44" height="39" border="0" alt="ePrints home" /></td>
          <td><a href="/" onMouseOut="MM_swapImgRestore()" onMouseOver="MM_swapImage('ePrints_banner_r6_c4','','/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r6_c4_f2.gif',1);"><img name="ePrints_banner_r6_c4" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r6_c4.gif" width="105" height="24" border="0" alt="ePrints home" /></a></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="1" height="24" border="0" alt="" /></td>
        </tr>
        <tr>
          <td><img name="ePrints_banner_r7_c2" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r7_c2.gif" width="104" height="15" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td colspan="8"><img name="ePrints_banner_r7_c4" src="/images/eprints/ePrints_banner_r7_c4.gif" width="517" height="15" border="0" alt="" /></td>
          <td><img src="/images/eprints/spacer.gif" width="1" height="15" border="0" alt="" /></td>
        </tr>
      </table></td>
  </tr>
    <tr><td><table width="100%" style="font-size: 90%; border: solid 1px #ccc; padding: 3px"><tr>
      <td align="left"><a href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/users/home">Login</a> | <a href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/register">Create Account</a></td>
      <td align="right" style="white-space: nowrap">
        <form method="get" accept-charset="utf-8" action="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/search" style="display:inline">
          <input class="ep_tm_searchbarbox" size="20" type="text" name="q" />
          <input class="ep_tm_searchbarbutton" value="Search" type="submit" name="_action_search" />
          <input type="hidden" name="_order" value="bytitle" />
          <input type="hidden" name="basic_srchtype" value="ALL" />
          <input type="hidden" name="_satisfyall" value="ALL" />
        </form>
      </td>
    </tr></table></td></tr>
  <tr>
    <td class="toplinks"><!-- InstanceBeginEditable name="content" -->


<div align="center">
  
  <table width="720" class="ep_tm_main"><tr><td align="left">
    <h1 class="ep_tm_pagetitle">Attention is Modulated by Motivational Relevance: A Behavioural and ERP Investigation of Affective Picture Processing</h1>
    <p style="margin-bottom: 1em" class="not_ep_block"><span class="person_name">Briggs, Kate Elizabeth</span> (2007) <xhtml:em>Attention is Modulated by Motivational Relevance: A Behavioural and ERP Investigation of Affective Picture Processing.</xhtml:em> PhD thesis, University of Tasmania.</p><p style="margin-bottom: 1em" class="not_ep_block"></p><table style="margin-bottom: 1em" class="not_ep_block"><tr><td valign="top" style="text-align:center"><a onmouseover="EPJS_ShowPreview( event, 'doc_preview_2877' );" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/1/01front_Kbriggs.pdf" onmouseout="EPJS_HidePreview( event, 'doc_preview_2877' );"><img alt="[img]" src="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/style/images/fileicons/application_pdf.png" class="ep_doc_icon" border="0" /></a><div class="ep_preview" id="doc_preview_2877"><table><tr><td><img alt="" src="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/thumbnails/1/preview.png" class="ep_preview_image" border="0" /><div class="ep_preview_title">Preview</div></td></tr></table></div></td><td valign="top"><a href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/1/01front_Kbriggs.pdf"><span class="ep_document_citation">PDF (Front Matter)</span></a> - Requires a PDF viewer<br />372Kb</td></tr><tr><td valign="top" style="text-align:center"><a onmouseover="EPJS_ShowPreview( event, 'doc_preview_2878' );" href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/2/02whole_kbriggs.pdf" onmouseout="EPJS_HidePreview( event, 'doc_preview_2878' );"><img alt="[img]" src="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/style/images/fileicons/application_pdf.png" class="ep_doc_icon" border="0" /></a><div class="ep_preview" id="doc_preview_2878"><table><tr><td><img alt="" src="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/thumbnails/2/preview.png" class="ep_preview_image" border="0" /><div class="ep_preview_title">Preview</div></td></tr></table></div></td><td valign="top"><a href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/2277/2/02whole_kbriggs.pdf"><span class="ep_document_citation">PDF (Whole Thesis)</span></a> - Requires a PDF viewer<br />4037Kb</td></tr></table><div class="not_ep_block"><h2>Abstract</h2><p style="padding-bottom: 16px; text-align: left; margin: 1em auto 0em auto">ERP evidence of affective picture processing generally agrees with one of two dominant&#13;
theories. The first is that enhanced ERP responses to pleasant and unpleasant stimuli&#13;
relative to neutral reflects the processing of stimulus motivational relevance, referred to&#13;
as the quadratic effect, and the second is that enhanced ERP responses to unpleasant&#13;
stimuli compared to pleasant and neutral stimuli reflects a negativity bias. The&#13;
overarching aims of the current series of empirical studies were to identify which of the&#13;
two aforementioned theories can most definitively account for affective picture&#13;
processing (Phase 1); and to investigate how processes of attentional engagement and&#13;
disengagement are influenced by the presence of motivationally relevant stimuli (Phase&#13;
2). Pictorial affective stimuli (high and low arousing pleasant, unpleasant, sexual, and&#13;
neutral stimuli) were presented in a modified oddball paradigm to 38 participants (19&#13;
male, 19 female) in Experiment 1 and 34 participants (17 male, 17 female) in Experiment&#13;
2. A negativity bias was demonstrated for P3b amplitude in Experiment 1; however&#13;
significantly enhanced P3b amplitudes evoked in response to sexually explicit stimuli in&#13;
Experiment 2 was not consistent with either dominant theory, and raised questions as to&#13;
the separable effects of motivational relevance and sexual arousal on cognitive processes.&#13;
Experiment 3 was aimed at investigating whether ERP responses are differentially&#13;
modulated by the social content of affective picture stimuli. The same participants from&#13;
Experiment 2 participated in Experiment 3 and the oddball task involved the presentation&#13;
of low arousing social and non-social pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral stimuli. No&#13;
significant differences in ERP component measures were shown between social and nonsocial&#13;
pleasant, or between social and non-social unpleasant stimuli, however both P2 and&#13;
P3b component amplitudes were enhanced in response to neutral faces compared with&#13;
neutral objects. Factors associated with facial recognition and difficulties extracting&#13;
affective information from a somewhat ambiguous neutral expression were cited as&#13;
possible explanations for the observed ERP component modulations.&#13;
The principal aim of Phase 2 was to investigate whether the presentation of&#13;
appetitive and aversive cues influences the engagement and disengagement components&#13;
of covert visual attention as inferred by responses to validly and invalidly cued targets&#13;
respectively. Participants in Experiment 4 @=I9 female) and Experiment 5 @=I8&#13;
female) were presented with a modified peripheral cueing paradigm, where pictorial&#13;
stimuli (sexual, mutilation, threatening, and neutral) served as peripheral cues. Target&#13;
processing as indexed by P1 and P3b amplitude showed significant facilitation in both&#13;
Experiments 4 and 5 when targets were cued by sexual and mutilation stimuli, regardless&#13;
of whether cueing was valid or invalid. It was therefore concluded that the engagement&#13;
and disengagement components of covert visual attention are not differentially affected&#13;
by motivationally relevant cues; rather, normal participants demonstrate a global&#13;
response bias when respondmg to targets that are cued by motivationally relevant&#13;
appetitive and aversive cues. The same participants from Experiment 5 were presented&#13;
with a peripheral cueing paradigm in Experiment 6, which aimed to investigate the effect&#13;
of phylogenetically (biological) and ontogenetically (cultural) fear-relevant stimuli on&#13;
processes of covert visual attention. Pictorial stimuli depicting dangerous animals, human&#13;
threat, and neutral objects served as peripheral cues. In line with preparedness theory&#13;
(Seligman 1970, 1971), target processing was facilitated by the presence of animal threat&#13;
stimuli compared to human threat and neutral stimuli, and also the early level of visual&#13;
processing as indexed by cue-evoked P 1 amplitude was enhanced in response to&#13;
phylogenetically, fear-relevant animal stimuli. A global response bias was again&#13;
demonstrated in Experiment 6, and it was concluded that the attentional system of normal&#13;
participants is sensitive to stimuli that have been evolutionarily associated with threat&#13;
and/or fear. The current dissertation therefore has theoretical implications for the&#13;
systematic study of affective picture processing. Furthermore, the introduction of a&#13;
peripheral cueing paradigm to the study of affective picture processing provides a new&#13;
insight into the effect that both appetitive and aversive stimuli have on processes of&#13;
attentional orienting and target processing.</p></div><table style="margin-bottom: 1em" cellpadding="3" class="not_ep_block" border="0"><tr><th valign="top" class="ep_row">Item Type:</th><td valign="top" class="ep_row">Thesis (PhD)</td></tr><tr><th valign="top" class="ep_row">Subjects:</th><td valign="top" class="ep_row"><a href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/view/subjects/380100.html">380000 Behavioural and Cognitive Sciences &gt; 380100 Psychology</a></td></tr><tr><th valign="top" class="ep_row">ID Code:</th><td valign="top" class="ep_row">2277</td></tr><tr><th valign="top" class="ep_row">Deposited By:</th><td valign="top" class="ep_row"><span class="ep_name_citation"><span class="person_name">MS Heather Excell</span></span></td></tr><tr><th valign="top" class="ep_row">Deposited On:</th><td valign="top" class="ep_row">22 Oct 2007 12:30</td></tr><tr><th valign="top" class="ep_row">Last Modified:</th><td valign="top" class="ep_row">09 Jan 2008 02:30</td></tr><tr><th valign="top" class="ep_row">ePrint Statistics:</th><td valign="top" class="ep_row"><a target="ePrintStats" href="/es/index.php?action=show_detail_eprint;id=2277;">View statistics for this ePrint</a></td></tr></table><p align="right">Repository Staff Only: <a href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/cgi/users/home?screen=EPrint::View&amp;eprintid=2277">item control page</a></p>
  </td></tr></table>
</div>



    <!-- InstanceEndEditable --></td>
  </tr>
  <tr>
    <td><!-- #BeginLibraryItem "/Library/footer_eprints.lbi" -->
    <table width="795" border="0" align="left" cellpadding="0" class="footer">
  <tr valign="top">
<td colspan="2"><div align="center"><a href="http://www.utas.edu.au">UTAS home</a> | <a href="http://www.utas.edu.au/library/">Library home</a> | <a href="/">ePrints home</a> | <a href="/contact.html">contact</a> | <a href="/information.html">about</a> | <a href="/view/">browse</a> | <a href="/perl/search/simple">search</a> | <a href="/perl/register">register</a> | <a href="/perl/users/home">user area</a> | <a href="/help/">help</a></div><br /></td>
</tr>
<tr><td colspan="2"><p><img src="/images/eprints/footerline.gif" width="100%" height="4" /></p></td></tr>
      <tr valign="top">
        <td width="68%" class="footer">Authorised by the University Librarian<br />
© University of Tasmania ABN 30 764 374 782<br />
      <a href="http://www.utas.edu.au/cricos/">CRICOS Provider Code 00586B</a> | <a href="http://www.utas.edu.au/copyright/copyright_disclaimers.html">Copyright &amp; Disclaimers</a> | <a href="http://www.utas.edu.au/accessibility/index.html">Accessibility</a> | <a href="http://eprints.utas.edu.au/feedback/">Site Feedback</a>  </td>
        <td width="32%"><div align="right">
            <p align="right" class="NoPrint"><a href="http://www.utas.edu.au/"><img src="http://www.utas.edu.au/shared/logos/unioftasstrip.gif" alt="University of Tasmania Home Page" width="260" height="16" border="0" align="right" /></a></p>
            <p align="right" class="NoPrint"><a href="http://www.utas.edu.au/"><br />
            </a></p>
        </div></td>
      </tr>
      <tr valign="top">
        <td><p>  </p></td>
        <td><div align="right"><span class="NoPrint"><a href="http://www.eprints.org/software/"><img src="/images/eprintslogo.gif" alt="ePrints logo" width="77" height="29" border="0" align="bottom" /></a></span></div></td>
      </tr>
    </table>
    <!-- #EndLibraryItem -->
    <div align="center"></div></td>
  </tr>
</table>

  </body>
</html>